Thought Disorder as a Neglected Dimension in Schizophrenia (2024)

As a library, NLM provides access to scientific literature. Inclusion in an NLM database does not imply endorsem*nt of, or agreement with, the contents by NLM or the National Institutes of Health.
Learn more: PMC Disclaimer | PMC Copyright Notice

Thought Disorder as a Neglected Dimension in Schizophrenia (1)

Link to Publisher's site

Alpha Psychiatry. 2022 Jan; 23(1): 5–11.

Published online 2021 Dec 30. doi:10.5152/alphapsychiatry.2021.21371

PMCID: PMC9674097

PMID: 36425242

Author information Article notes Copyright and License information PMC Disclaimer

Abstract

Thought disorder (TD) is one of the main symptom domains of schizophrenia. TD is a construct that can be seen in both acute and chronic periods in patients with schizophrenia. It is associated with a poor prognosis and relapses, and it disrupts social-occupational functioning. TD also shows a strong familial aggregation and increases the risk of transition to schizophrenia in subjects with ultra-high risk. Although TD was once thought to be pathognomonic to schizophrenia, now it is known that it is a transdiagnostic symptom that is seen not only in schizophrenia but also in other psychiatric disorders. This article reviews the concept of thought disorder in terms of its history, clinical evaluation, neurocognitive features and clinical course in patients with schizophrenia.

Keywords: Prognosis, psychosocial functioning, remission, schizophrenia, thought

Introduction

Thought disorder (TD) has long been considered as one of the main symptom domains of schizophrenia.1 TD is a multidimensional construct, as it refers to thinking, language, and communication abnormalities.2

TD is found in many patients with schizophrenia during exacerbation of the psychosis. TD symptoms related to acute episodes are reduced by antipsychotic medication, however, it has been shown that residual symptoms continued even after clinical remission has been achieved.1,3 Additionally, it has also been suggested that the continuation of the severe TD after the acute period may indicate a worse prognosis in schizophrenia.1 TD seems to predict relapse, and this predictive value is related to the presence of the symptoms in the early stages of the illness.4 Another important issue that has been associated with TD is therapeutic alliance; TD can interfere with verbal communication and, therefore, leads to disruption of treatment.5

Considering the studies carried out so far, it is concluded that a relationship exists between TD and social-occupational functioning. Higher rates of readmission to hospital and unemployment were observed in patients with TD during follow-up.1 Also, TD has been associated with a decrease in work performance6 and worse social functioning in patients with schizophrenia.7,8

TD was formerly thought to be specific to schizophrenia. However, we now know that language peculiarities such as clanging, associative loosening, blocking, or skipping from topic to topic not only happen in schizophrenia but also in other psychiatric disorders including mania and depression. People who do not have a psychiatric diagnosis can also show these features, especially when they are distressed or tired.9,10

History of Thought Disorder

It was first mentioned by Esquirol11 in the first half of the 19th century that in psychiatric disorders, pathology may be manifested by a disorder in coordinating thoughts. In the same period, Prichard first used the term “incoherence” for defects observed in the connections between thoughts.12 Guislain13 coined the term “incoherence des ideées” (encoherence of thoughts) for TD, referring to the distinction between thought and speech disorders.

For the first time in 1867, Griesinger distinguished disorders related to the form and content of thought. Then, Séglas distinguished TD from speech and language disorders by saying that variations in speech can be subdivided into 3 categories such as dyslogies (TDs), dysphasia (language disorders), or dislocations (speech disorders). Séglas also mentioned that the TD, which he named “dyslogy,” has 4 subtypes namely tempo, form, syntax, and content.14

In the early 20th century, with Kraepelin introducing dementia praecox as a diagnostic construct, TDs played an important role in the conceptualization of psychosis. He argued that schizophrenia is fundamentally a TD.15 In 1910, Kraepelin16 used the term “akataphasia” to refer to TD as a common linguistic manifestation symptom in patients with dementia praecox. He described akataphasia as an inability to find the appropriate expression for a thought.14

In 1911, Bleuler,17 a Swiss psychiatrist was the first author to use the term “schizophrenia” to describe “splitting of psychic functions.” In his opinion “loosening of association” was pathognomonic of schizophrenia and disruption in associations was causing illogicality and incoherence.2 Bleuler17 mentioned the idiosyncratic structure of the connections between thoughts in schizophrenia and argued that concepts were expressed in a distorted manner as a result of concentration, displacement, and symbolism.14

Carl Schneider also described concepts, such as derailment, fusion, suspension, and driveling. In his opinion, “constancy, organization, and continuity” components that exist in normal thinking are impaired in schizophrenic thought.14,18

Difficulties exist in naming the concepts of TD phenomenologically. Over time, the same pathology has been expressed by different authors with different terms. Concepts such as “incoherence” by Prichard, “loosening of association” by Bleuler, and “derailment” by Carl Schneider might all point to the same pathology.14

Silvano Arieti has comprehensively described the system of thought that produces delusion. While the human brain changes from concrete to abstract in the evolutionary process, concrete forms of thought reappear in schizophrenia. This thinking system, which dominates among children, in dreams and during psychosis, has been named “paleological thought,” since it belongs to the earlier stages of social and mental development. Three main features distinguish paleological thought from Aristotelian thought. The laws of identity, contradiction, and excluded middle are canceled in paleological thinking. Patients with schizophrenia tend to think in a paleological way based on common qualities. According to the law of identity in paleological thinking, it is sufficient for 2 objects to have one common feature for them to be identical. For example, a patient thinks that she is the Virgin Mary because she is a virgin and so was Mary.19

An important development in the historical process of the TD concept is that recent approaches question the unitary structure of TD and its specificity to schizophrenia. Nancy Andreasen,2,9 a researcher who has made significant contributions to this field, criticized the assessment of TD as a unitary dimension and argued that the disorders seen under the concept of TD should be regrouped under the name of thought, language, and communication disorders. Andreasen2,9 disagreed with the view that it is specific to schizophrenia, and states that it can be seen in other psychiatric illnesses and may not even be seen in many patients with schizophrenia.

TD is still considered as a unitary concept in The Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (DSM) and The International Classification of Diseases (ICD) (the World Health Organization’s International Classification of Diseases) diagnostic systems. DSM-IV has named TD “disorganized speech,” and this definition is preserved in DSM-5.20,21 Notably, this approach ignores the complex nature and importance of TD and reduces its importance in the diagnosis of schizophrenia.15 Indeed, TD has been studied by many researchers, and it seems impossible to explain it with a single model.

Thought Disorder

Different terms such as TD, formal TD (FTD), language disorder, speech disorder, and communication disorder are used to describe in the literature.

TD is divided into 2 subcategories: disorders of thought form and disorders of thought content. Thought content disorders are determined by evaluating the meanings of thoughts. The most prominent thought content disorder seen in schizophrenia is delusions. There are overvalued thoughts at some point between normal thinking and delusion. Obsessions, mystical and metaphysical thoughts also reflect abnormal thought content.22

FTDs are defined by any deficiency of organizing words, concepts, phrases, and ideas in a logical order to express a certain purpose. This deficiency can be grouped under 2 main headings: negative and positive FTDs. Negative TD is identified by poverty of speech, alogia, and poverty in the content of speech, while positive TD is defined by circ*mstantiality, tangentiality, derailment, clanging, neologism, illogicality, perseveration, blocking, and incoherence.9

Andreasen10 stated that the most common TDs were derailment, loss of goal, poverty of content, and tangentiality in patients with schizophrenia, but these are not all equally pathological. Poverty of speech, poverty of content, tangentiality, derailment, incoherence, pressure of speech, distractible speech, illogicality, clanging, neologisms, and word approximations have been listed as more pathological types of TD.10,23

Clinical Assessment of Thought Disorder

General psychopathological scales, which are used to determine the severity of symptoms in the clinical setting (e.g., scale for the assessment of positive symptoms [SAPS], scale for the assessment of negative symptoms [SANS], the positive and negative syndrome scale [PANSS] for schizophrenia) contain only a few items to evaluate positive and negative TD.24

Various FTD scales exist. One of the widely used clinician-rated assessment scales is the “Thought, Language, and Communication Disorders (TLC) scale” developed by Andreasen9 in 1979. It is a measure of positive and negative TD, based on 18 symptoms of FTD.10 However, the subjective symptoms are not coded, and this scale does not include all FTD symptoms.24 It is also a more clinical scale that reflects explicit pathology and is quite insensitive to subtle abnormalities in the speech of relatives of patients with schizophrenia.25

The Thought Disorder Index (TDI) is another rating scale for TD. It was developed by Johnston and Holzman in 197926 and includes 23 TD categories. TDI scores are usually obtained from verbal responses to the Rorschach Test and verbal subscales of the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Test.27 In contrast to the thought, language, and communication scale, it was designed to also measure mild level disorders.28 However, the TDI is time-consuming, and a well-trained rater is needed for assessment.27

The bizarre-idiosyncratic thinking was developed by Marengo et al29 in 1986 to measure positive TD, and it focuses only on positive FTD. This measure comprises of 5 categories which are linguistic form and structure, content of statement, intermixing, relationship between question and response, and behavior. It has been used to assess the severity of TD and longitudinal changes in TD symptoms over time.29

In Chen et al30 developed a scale considering psycholinguistic features. Clinical Language (CLANG) considers 17 symptoms classified according to the linguistic structure. This scale evaluates 3 different types of language disorders: syntax, semantics, and production. It also includes a detailed examination of sound quality and articulation and speech style, not just thought or discourse. So, it allows fluency, voice quality, and articulation to be evaluated.23

The Thought and Language Index (TLI) developed by Liddle et al28 in 2002 consists of impoverishment of thought and disorganization of thought subscales and evaluates 8 abnormalities of speech. The participant is asked to produce eight 1-min speech samples in response to the pictures of Rorschach31 or thematic apperception test.32 Then, recorded verbal transcripts are assessed. This scale is a reliable measure of thought and language disorders which is also sensitive to subtle impairments.28

The Thought and Language Disorder (TALD) scale is a comprehensive measure developed by Kircher et al33 in 2014. The scale consists of 30 FTD symptoms and distinguishes objective and subjective FTD symptoms. Evaluation is made during the 50-min clinical interview and includes 2 parts. In the first part of the interview, individuals are asked about common subjects (e.g., daily issues, hobbies, etc.) A semi-structured interview is then conducted. TALD can also be used in the evaluation of prodromal patients and relatives with self-experienced FTD.33

The Dokuz Eylül University Thought Disorder Scale (DTDS) was developed by Yalınçetin et al34 in 2020. This scale consists of a 3-factor structure. The first factor is positive TD (PosTD) and includes accelerated thought process, disturbance in staying on topic, loosening in thought process, idiosyncratic word use, and illogical thinking. The second factor is negative TD (NegTD), which is comprised of poverty of speech, poverty in content of speech, and disruption in the flow of speech. The third factor is pragmatic comprehension disorder, and 2 subtitles are examined in this area: understanding the intents of others and comprehension of abstract expressions. Stories containing metaphor, irony, and faux pas are used to assess understanding of abstract expression. According to the results of the validity and reliability study, the Cronbach α value of the whole scale was found to be 0.86. A high degree of correlation was found between the DTDS PosTD subscale items and the SAPS positive formal TD items (P < .001). A high degree of correlation was also found between the items of the DTDS NegTD subscale and the SANS-Alogy items (P < .001). DTDS is an original scale that can be used to evaluate TD in schizophrenia. This work has not been published yet. One of the advantages of this scale is that it is practical as it is based on the clinical interview, similar to SANS, SAPS, and PANSS, unlike many other scales found in the literature. Another strength of DTDS is that it assesses the PosDB and NegDB dimensions as clustered, condensing these dimensions and scoring hierarchically for severity. The fact that it includes pragmatic comprehension disorders brings a novelty to the literature, differing from other scales.

In summary, the weakness of TLC is that it misses mild disturbances. Unlike TLC, TDI and TLI are susceptible to subtle disturbances. Although TDI has the advantage of detecting subtle abnormalities, it is a very time-consuming scale for routine use, and its scoring requires extensive training. TALD can also be used to detect subtle abnormalities for both prodromal patients and relatives. Another advantage of TALD is that it distinguishes objective and subjective FTD and provides a multidimensional assessment opportunity. Differing from other scales, CLANG is a scale that could also measure speech style.

When the tools developed to measure TDs are examined, it is evident that the notion of TD is limited only to positive and negative TD and mostly focuses on the positive dimension. Comprehensive scales for both posTD and negTD take time due to the long application and evaluation process.

Thought Disorder in Populations at High-Risk for Schizophrenia

Studies of high-risk psychosis states have become increasingly important in recent years. These studies aimed to determine the risk factors that can predict transition to schizophrenia or another psychotic disorder. FTD is one of the features that increase the risk of transition to psychosis.35 FTD may be an endophenotypic marker. People at ultra-high risk for psychosis have TDs similar to those with psychotic disorders, but less severe.36 In a study that evaluated 100 individuals with clinically high risk of psychosis for 2.5 years, it was shown that subthreshold TD tends to persist and lead to the onset of psychosis.37 These findings are in the same line with the clustering of FTD in relatives of patients with schizophrenia, and it suggests that genetic factors may play a role in the basis of TD.24,38 Especially deviant verbalizations, peculiar word use, and verbal fluency are seen prominently in first degree relatives of individuals with schizophrenia.38-41 In another longitudinal study of offspring of parents with schizophrenia or affective disorder and normal controls, it was found that negative FTD is related to schizophrenia to a greater degree, whereas positive FTD is associated with affective symptoms.42 This could mean that negative FTD is better at predicting conversion to schizophrenia.

A study that combined automated speech analyses with machine learning to predict later psychosis onset in youths at clinical high-risk for psychosis found that psychosis could be predicted. Although the sample size was small in this initial study, these novel speech measures provided important information about the potential predictive capacity.43

Thought Disorder in First-Episode Psychosis

FTD is a powerful predictor of the transition of patients from first-episode psychosis (FEP) to schizophrenia.44 Therefore, it is important to determine the subtypes and reveal their relations with the clinical course.

One study that compared an FEP sample to a healthy control group for subtypes of FTD found that FEP patients had notable thought abnormalities compared with healthy controls in terms of poverty of speech, perseveration, and peculiar word use. Additionally, although the impoverishment in thought processes decreased with an increase in the years of education in FEP patients, no relationship was found between the increase in the years of education and disorganization of thought.45

In another study with patients with FEP, disorganization of thought was the only dimension of FTD associated with functional outcome, especially with social function. Moreover, disorganization of thought in first-episode psychosis also predicted the higher number of hospitalizations.46

Effect of Thought Disorder on Prognosis of Schizophrenia

The course of TD varies widely among patients. In some schizophrenia patients, TD continues irrespective of the course of psychosis.47 This finding is consistent with the view that TD is a semi-independent dimension in schizophrenia.48

The presence of TD, especially negTD from the early stages of schizophrenia, has a poor prognostic value and also is predictive for relapse.4 The probability of going into remission is lower in patients with negTD.49

NegTD is more persistent than posTD in a clinical course. Although FTD can predict the outcome, types of TD differ according to their prognostic effects. Even if positive forms of TD are not related to the course and chronicity of the disease; negTD, especially poverty of speech and poverty of content, might be predictive for poor ­prognosis.4,49,50

However, in an earlier study, it was stated that posTD could also ­predict prognosis. It has been shown that severe TD that persists after an acute episode shows a poor clinical course and low functionality and predicts subsequent delusions. In conclusion, to predict the severity of illness, TD that continues after the acute period is an indicator.1

PosTD seems to be more severe during acute episodes. Remission can be observed after an acute episode.49 Although antipsychotic therapy reduces the thought impairment concerning acute episodes, the milder residual thought pathology perseveres even after clinical remission is reached. Less severe TD benefits less from antipsychotic treatment than severe TD, as they are selective to the type of thought pathology rather than the degree of severity.3

Poverty of speech and peculiar logic have been found to be ­associated with social functioning and symptomatic remission. These areas can distinguish schizophrenia patients with and without remission.8

Moreover, various forms of TDS were not seen with the same frequency in different diagnostic categories. Relative to bipolar disorder and schizoaffective disorder, patients of schizophrenia had poverty of speech and thought content more frequently.50 This situation is not surprising given the difference in the courses of the diseases. While positive thinking disorder is more prominent in patients with mania, negative thinking disorder is more prominent in patients with schizophrenia. Considering the subcategories of TD, fluency and productivity of language behavior are useful areas to distinguish between affective psychosis and schizophrenia.49

Impact of Thought Disorder on Psychosocial Functioning

In serious psychiatric illnesses such as schizophrenia, social and occupational functioning are as important as the symptoms of the illness. These are perhaps even more important, as social functioning is the most important area that determines one’s participation in life. Functioning may not improve even if symptoms improve.51

Communication skills of the patients may be impaired due to FTD.9 Thought is an important part of communication, and communication is an important part of social functioning. Therefore, interpersonal communication skills and, accordingly social functioning are affected as expected in the presence of TD.6

As mentioned, patients of schizophrenia have poverty of speech and peculiar logic, which are in an intimate relationship with social functioning. It has also been shown that poverty of speech, as well as goal weakening, are associated with aggressive behaviors. Social beneficial activities, disturbing and aggressive behaviors, personal and social relationships, and self-care are subdivisions of social functioning; and the only TLI component related to these subdivisions is speech poverty.8

It has been found that disconnected speech has a relationship with social politeness; however, verbal underproductivity is associated with social engagement and fewer friendships among chronically hospitalized schizophrenia patients.52 In the same direction, it has been shown that in individuals with schizophrenia living in the community, verbal underproductivity predicts social behavior rated by the observer, while disconnected speech predicts socially acceptable behavior.7 In a study examining the relationship between TD in schizophrenia patients and quality of life index of them, it was found that verbal underproductivity (negTD) affects daily functioning and social contact, whereas pressured speech (posTD) influences satisfaction with life. Verbal fluency is thought to be an important factor that predicts daily problem-solving skills.53,54 Given the relationship between quality of life and verbal underproductivity, it may be an appropriate target in schizophrenia rehabilitation.55 Moreover, an 8-year follow-up study showed that positive FTD predicted the patients’ occupational functioning years later.6

In a recent study evaluating the relationship of FTD with cognitive impairment and functionality, TD has been evaluated in 4 dimensions as positive, negative, objective, and subjective. It has been shown that a relationship exists between objective positive FTD and social functionality, and between objective negative FTD and physical quality of life, and social functioning. Besides, subjective negative FTD was found to be strongly correlated with quality of life. Since recovery in schizophrenia cannot be considered independently of functionality, psychosocial treatments should focus more specifically on different dimensions of TD.56

Relation Between Thought Disorder and Cognitive Dysfunctions in Schizophrenia

The mechanisms of TD are still not fully elucidated.15 Many studies have shown that there is a relationship between cognitive deficits and TD in schizophrenia.57,58

In many studies to date, a relationship has been established between FTD and executive function.58-60 Although the specific basis of posTD and negTD in schizophrenia is unclear, and some findings suggest that positive and negative FTD might be related to different cognitive domains.56,58

A recent meta-analysis found that a significant association exists between both positive and negative FTD and neurocognitive deficits, executive dysfunction, and semantic processing abnormalities in schizophrenia. However, syntactic deficiencies were more specific to positive FTD, and executive functions also differed in different types of TD, supporting that the neurobiological basis of negative FTD and positive FTD are probably different.61 The findings of another study showed that objective positive FTD was associated with deficits in executive functions, while objective negative FTD was associated with deficits in all cognitive domains, consistent with this meta-analysis. These findings support the idea that different subgroups of FTD in schizophrenia may be associated with different cognitive impairments.56

The results of imaging studies in patients with schizophrenia support the relationship between neurocognition and TD. PosFTD was related to aberrations in the anterior cingulate cortex and orbitofrontal cortex, and volume reductions in both Broca’s and Wernicke’s areas, while negFTD was associated with decreases in the medial frontal/orbitofrontal cortex and abnormalities in fronto-cingular and striatal abnormalities. As these findings show, there may be pathology in brain regions related to language and executive functions in schizophrenia, and understanding this relationship may enable us to better comprehend TD.24,61-63

Conclusion

TD is a multidimensional construct that can be seen in some psychiatric conditions and healthy individuals and refers to thinking, language, and communication abnormalities.

TD, mostly negTD, is related to poor prognosis in schizophrenia and tends to be more permanent in the clinical course than posTD. Furthermore, persistent TD is associated with poor social and occupational functioning and could predict relapse. Its negative impact on therapeutic alliance and its relationship with re-hospitalization should not be forgotten. For these reasons, clinical studies for the treatment of FTD are needed.

As TD occurs not only in patients with schizophrenia but also in their relatives, it is thought to be an important genetic component. In line with this, FTD also enhances the risk of transition to schizophrenia in subjects with ultra-high risk. Even if not alone, when evaluated together with other psychosis risk factors, it can be useful in detecting future psychosis. A combination of automated speech analyses with machine learning could predict later psychosis onset in youths at clinical high risk for psychosis.

Many scales have been developed that assess TD from different aspects including linguistics, posTD, negTD, and pragmatic disorder. It takes time to make an assessment covering all areas. Considering the studies in the literature, the heterogeneity in the use of the scales makes it difficult to evaluate the results together. More widespread use of computerized tools for TD analysis may help to eliminate these problems. They also have some important advantages such as being unbiased, objective, repeatable, and faster. Using automated speech analysis such as computational natural language methods, complex networks, and quantitative linguistics to evaluate speech are some of the new and promising approaches.64

Considering the relationship of TD with functioning and recovery in schizophrenia, it is very important to elucidate the pathogenesis of FTD more precisely. Increasing evidence on the relationship between TD and neurocognitive features indicates that neuroimaging studies can contribute to this field.

TD is now thought to be a transdiagnostic symptom that is seen not only in schizophrenia but also in other psychiatric illnesses and even in healthy individuals. Determining how TD domains are affected in different psychiatric diseases will contribute to our understanding of the pathophysiology of diseases. Considering the transdiagnostic approach, it will be useful to identify clusters of overlap with other psychiatric diseases.

Funding Statement

The authors declared that this study has received no financial support.

Footnotes

Peer Review: Externally peer-reviewed.

Author Contributions: Concept - S.U.Ö., K.A.; Design - S.U.Ö., K.A.; Supervision - K.A.; Data Collection and/or Processing - S.U.Ö.; Literature Review - S.U.Ö.; Writing - S.U.Ö., K.A.; Critical Review - S.U.Ö., K.A.

Conflict of Interest: The authors have no conflict of interest to declare.

References

1. Harrow M, Marengo JT. Schizophrenic thought disorder at followup: its persistence and prognostic significance. Schizophr Bull. 1986;12(3):373–393.. 10.1093/schbul/12.3.373) [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

2. Andreasen NC.Should the term “thought disorder” be revised?Compr Psychiatry. 1982;23(4):291–299.. 10.1016/0010-440X(82)90079-7) [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

3. Spohn HE Coyne L Larson J, et al. Episodic and residual thought pathology in chronic schizophrenics: effect of neuroleptics. Schizophr Bull. 1986;12(3):394–407.. 10.1093/schbul/12.3.394) [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

4. Wilcox JA.Thought disorder and relapse in schizophrenia. Psychopathology. 1990;23(3):153–156.. 10.1159/000284652) [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

5. Cavelti M, Homan P, Vauth R. The impact of thought disorder on therapeutic alliance and personal recovery in schizophrenia and schizoaffective disorder: an exploratory study. Psychiatry Res. 2016;239:92–98.. 10.1016/j.psychres.2016.02.070) [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

6. Racenstein JM, Penn D, Harrow M, Schleser R. Thought disorder and psychosocial functioning in schizophrenia: the concurrent and predictive relationships. J Nerv Ment Dis. 1999;187(5):281–289.. 10.1097/00005053-199905000-00003) [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

7. Bowie CR, Gupta M, Holshausen K. Disconnected and underproductive speech in schizophrenia: unique relationships across multiple indicators of social functioning. Schizophr Res. 2011;131(1-3):152–156.. 10.1016/j.schres.2011.04.014) [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

8. Yalınçetin B, Ulaş H, Var L.et al. Relation of formal thought disorder to symptomatic remission and social functioning in schizophrenia. Compr Psychiatry. 2016;70:98–104.. 10.1016/j.comppsych.2016.07.001) [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

9. Andreasen NC.Thought, language, and communication disorders I. Clinical assessment, definition of terms, and evaluation of their reliability. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1979;36(12):1315–1321.. 10.1001/archpsyc.1979.01780120045006) [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

10. Andreasen NC.Scale for the assessment of thought, language, and communication (TLC). Schizophr Bull. 1986;12(3):473–482.. 10.1093/schbul/12.3.473) [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

11. Esquirol E.Des Maladies Mentales Considérées sous les rapports médical, Hygiénique et Médico-Legal. Paris: Baillière; 1838. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

12. Berrios G.The History of Mental Symptoms - Descriptive Psychopathology Since the Nineteenth Century. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press; 1996, pp. . 71–84.. [Google Scholar]

13. Guislain J.Leçons orales sur les phrénopathies: ou, Traité théorique et pratique des maladies mentales. Gand: L. Hebbelynck; 1852. [Google Scholar]

14. Jerónimo J, Queirós T, Cheniaux E, Telles-Correia D. Formal thought disorders - Historical roots. Front Psychiatry. 2018;9(V):572. 10.3389/fpsyt.2018.00572) [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

15. Hart M, Lewine RRJ. Rethinking thought disorder. Schizophr Bull. 2017;43(3):514–522.. 10.1093/schbul/sbx003) [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

16. Kraepelin E.Dementia Praecox and Paraphrenia. 8th ed. Edinburgh: Livingstone; 1919. [Google Scholar]

17. Bleuler E.Dementia praecox or the group of schizophrenias. In: Zinkin J.ed. Dementia Praecox or the Group of Schizophrenias. New York, NY: International Universities Press; 1950. [Google Scholar]

18. Babco*ck H.Dementia Praecox. New York: Science Press; 1933. [Google Scholar]

19. Arieti S.Interpretation of Schizophrenia. New York: Basic Books Publishers; 1974:215–381.. [Google Scholar]

20. American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders: DSM-IV-TR. Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association; 2000. 10.1016/S2215-0366(18)30059-2 [Google Scholar]

21. American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (5th ed.). Arlington, VA: Author; 2013. 10.1016/S2215-0366(18)30059-2 [Google Scholar]

22. Cutting J., Murphy D.Schizophrenic thought disorder: a psychological and organic interpretation. Br J Psychiatry. 1988;152:310–319.. 10.1192/bjp.152.3.310) [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

23. Covington MA, He C, Brown C.et al. Schizophrenia and the structure of language: the linguist’s view. Schizophr Res. 2005;77(1):85–98.. 10.1016/j.schres.2005.01.016) [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

24. Kircher T, Bröhl H, Meier F, Engelen J. Formal thought disorders: from phenomenology to neurobiology. Lancet Psychiatry. 2018;5(6):515–526.. 10.1016/S2215-0366(18)30059-2 [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

25. Docherty NM.On identifying the processes underlying schizophrenic speech disorder. Schizophr Bull. 2012;38(6):1327–1335.. 10.1093/schbul/sbr048) [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

26. Johnston MH, Holzman PS.Assessing Schizophrenic Thinking: A Clinical and Research Instrument for Measuring Thought Disorder. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass. 1979. [Google Scholar]

27. Solovay MR, Shenton ME, Gasperetti C.et al. Scoring manual for the Thought Disorder Index. Schizophr Bull. 1986;12(3):483–496.. 10.1093/schbul/12.3.483. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

28. Liddle PF, Ngan ETC, Caissie SL.et al. Thought and language index: an instrument for assessing thought and language in schizophrenia. Br J Psychiatry. 2002;181:326–330.. 10.1192/bjp.181.4.326) [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

29. Marengo JT, Harrow M, Lanin-Kettering I, Wilson A. Evaluating bizarre-idiosyncratic thinking: a comprehensive index of positive thought disorder. Schizophr Bull. 1986;12(3):497–511.. 10.1093/schbul/12.3.497) [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

30. Chen EYH, Lam LCW, Kan CS.et al. Language disorganisation in schizophrenia: validation and assessment with a new clinical rating instrument. Hong Kong J Psychiatr. 1996;6(1):4–13.. [Google Scholar]

31. Rorschach H.Psychodiagnostics: A Diagnostic Test Based on Perception. New York: Grune & Stratton; 1942. [Google Scholar]

32. Murray HA.Manual for the Thematic Apperception Test. Cam- Bridge. MA: Harvard University Press; 1943. [Google Scholar]

33. Kircher T, Krug A, Stratmann M.et al. A rating scale for the assessment of objective and subjective formal thought and language disorder (TALD). Schizophr Res. 2014;160(1-3):216–221.. 10.1016/j.schres.2014.10.024) [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

34. Yalincetin B, Bora E, Akdede BB, Alptekin K. A new assessment tool for formal thought disorder in schizophrenia: Dokuz Eylul Thought Disorder Scale. Schizophr Bull. 2020;46(suppl 1):171. [Google Scholar]

35. Sabb FW, van Erp TGM, Hardt ME.et al. Language network dysfunction as a predictor of outcome in youth at clinical high risk for psychosis. Schizophr Res. 2010;116(2-3):173–183.. 10.1016/j.schres.2009.09.042) [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

36. Demjaha A, Weinstein S, Stahl D.et al. Formal thought disorder in people at ultra-high risk of psychosis. BJPsych Open. 2017;3(4):165–170.. 10.1192/bjpo.bp.116.004408) [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

37. DeVylder JE, Muchomba FM, Gill KE.et al. Symptom trajectories and psychosis onset in a clinical high-risk cohort: the relevance of subthreshold thought disorder. Schizophr Res. 2014;159(2-3):278–283.. 10.1016/j.schres.2014.08.008) [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

38. Levy DL, Coleman MJ, Sung H.et al. The genetic basis of thought disorder and language and communication disturbances in schizophrenia. J Neurolinguist. 2010;23(3):176. 10.1016/j.jneuroling.2009.08.003) [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

39. Morgan CJ, Coleman MJ, Ulgen A.et al. Thought disorder in schizophrenia and bipolar disorder probands, their relatives, and nonpsychiatric controls. Schizophr Bull. 2017;43(3):523–535.. 10.1093/schbul/sbx016) [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

40. Baskak B, Ozel ET, Atbasoglu EC, Baskak SC. Peculiar word use as a possible trait marker in schizophrenia. Schizophr Res. 2008;103(1-3):311–317.. 10.1016/j.schres.2008.04.025) [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

41. Laurent A, Biloa-Tang M, Bougerol T.et al. Executive/attentional performance and measures of schizotypy in patients with schizophrenia and in their nonpsychotic first-degree relatives. Schizophr Res. 2000;46(2-3):269–283.. 10.1016/S0920-9964(99)00232-7) [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

42. Ott SL, Roberts S, Rock D, Allen J, Erlenmeyer-Kimling L. L positive and negative thought disorder and psychopathology in childhood among subjects with adulthood schizophrenia. Schizophr Res. 2002;58(2-3):231–239.. 10.1016/S0920-9964(01)00339-5) [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

43. Bedi G, Carrillo F, Cecchi GA.et al. Automated analysis of free speech predicts psychosis onset in high-risk youths. npj Schizophr. 2015;1(1):15030. 10.1038/npjschz.2015.30) [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

44. Rusaka M, Rancans E. Preliminary results of a prospective follow-up study of first episode acute transient psychotic disorder in Latvia. Eur Neuropsychopharmacol. 2012;22:S306–S307.. 10.1016/S0924-977X(12)70468-6) [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

45. Ayer A, Yalınçetin B, Aydınlı E.et al. Formal thought disorder in first-episode psychosis. Compr Psychiatry. 2016;70:209–215.. 10.1016/j.comppsych.2016.08.005) [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

46. Roche E, Lyne J, O’Donoghue B.et al. The prognostic value of formal thought disorder following first episode psychosis. Schizophr Res. 2016;178(1-3):29–34.. 10.1016/j.schres.2016.09.017) [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

47. Marengo JT, Harrow M. Longitudinal courses of thought disorder in schizophrenia and schizoaffective disorder. Schizophr Bull. 1997;23(2):273–285.. 10.1093/schbul/23.2.273) [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

48. Parnas J, Jørgensen A, Teasdale TW, Schulsinger F, Mednick SA. Temporal course of symptoms and social functioning in relapsing schizophrenics: a 6-year follow-up. Compr Psychiatry. 1988;29(4):361–371.. 10.1016/0010-440X(88)90017-X) [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

49. Andreasen NC, Grove WM. Thought, language, and communication in schizophrenia: diagnosis and prognosis. Schizophr Bull. 1986;12(3):348–359.. 10.1093/schbul/12.3.348) [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

50. Wilcox J, Winokur G, Tsuang M. Predictive value of thought disorder in new-onset psychosis. Compr Psychiatry. 2012;53(6):674–678.. 10.1016/j.comppsych.2011.12.002) [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

51. Robinson DG, Woerner MG, McMeniman M, Mendelowitz A, Bilder RM. Symptomatic and functional recovery from a first episode of schizophrenia or schizoaffective disorder. Am J Psychiatry. 2004;161(3):473–479.. 10.1176/appi.ajp.161.3.473) [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

52. Bowie CR, Harvey PD. Communication abnormalities predict functional outcomes in chronic schizophrenia: differential associations with social and adaptive functions. Schizophr Res. 2008;103(1-3):240–247.. 10.1016/j.schres.2008.05.006) [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

53. Holshausen K, Harvey PD, Elvevåg B, Foltz PW, Bowie CR. Latent semantic variables are associated with formal thought disorder and adaptive behavior in older inpatients with schizophrenia. Cortex. 2014;55(1):88–96.. 10.1016/j.cortex.2013.02.006) [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

54. Revheim N, Schechter I, Kim D.et al. Neurocognitive and symptom correlates of daily problem-solving skills in schizophrenia. Schizophr Res. 2006;83(2-3):237–245.. 10.1016/j.schres.2005.12.849) [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

55. Tan EJ, Thomas N, Rossell SL. Speech disturbances and quality of life in schizophrenia: differential impacts on functioning and life satisfaction. Compr Psychiatry. 2014;55(3):693–698.. 10.1016/j.comppsych.2013.10.016) [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

56. Mutlu E, Abaoğlu H, Barışkın E.et al. The cognitive aspect of formal thought disorder and its relationship with global social functioning and the quality of life of in schizophrenia. Soc Psychiatry Psychiatr Epidemiol. 2021. 10.1007/s00127-021-02024-w) [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

57. Kerns JG, Berenbaum H. Cognitive impairments associated with formal thought disorder in people with schizophrenia. J Abnorm Psychol. 2002;111(2):211–224.. 10.1037/0021-843X.111.2.211) [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

58. Nagels A, Fährmann P, Stratmann M.et al. Distinct neuropsychological correlates in positive and negative formal thought disorder syndromes: the thought and language disorder scale in endogenous psychoses. Neuropsychobiology. 2016;73(3):139–147.. 10.1159/000441657) [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

59. Stirling J, Hellewell J, Blakey A, Deakin W. Thought disorder in schizophrenia is associated with both executive dysfunction and circ*mscribed impairments in semantic function. Psychol Med. 2006;36(4):475–484.. 10.1017/S0033291705006884) [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

60. Barrera A, McKenna PJ, Berrios GE. Formal thought disorder in schizophrenia: an executive or a semantic deficit?Psychol Med. 2005;35(1):121–132.. 10.1017/S003329170400279X) [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

61. Bora E, Yalincetin B, Akdede BB, Alptekin K. Neurocognitive and linguistic correlates of positive and negative formal thought disorder: a meta-analysis. Schizophr Res. 2019;209:2–11.. 10.1016/j.schres.2019.05.025) [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

62. Sans-Sansa B, McKenna PJ, Canales-Rodríguez EJ.et al. Association of formal thought disorder in schizophrenia with structural brain abnormalities in language-related cortical regions. Schizophr Res. 2013;146(1-3):308–313.. 10.1016/j.schres.2013.02.032) [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

63. Horn H, Federspiel A, Wirth M.et al. Gray matter volume differences specific to formal thought disorder in schizophrenia. Psychiatry Res. 2010;182(2):183–186.. 10.1016/j.pscychresns.2010.01.016) [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

64. Elvevåg B, Foltz PW, Rosenstein M.et al. Thoughts about disordered thinking: measuring and quantifying the laws of order and disorder. Schizophr Bull. 2017;43(3):509–513.. 10.1093/schbul/sbx040) [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

Articles from Alpha Psychiatry are provided here courtesy of AVES

Thought Disorder as a Neglected Dimension in Schizophrenia (2024)
Top Articles
Brushing Up For Shedding Season
What Is “Undue Hardship” in Student Loan Bankruptcy? - Oaktree Law
Sdn Md 2023-2024
Kmart near me - Perth, WA
Winston Salem Nc Craigslist
Blanchard St Denis Funeral Home Obituaries
Ross Dress For Less Hiring Near Me
Z-Track Injection | Definition and Patient Education
Linkvertise Bypass 2023
Costco The Dalles Or
Doublelist Paducah Ky
Craigslist Furniture Bedroom Set
Alpha Kenny Buddy - Songs, Events and Music Stats | Viberate.com
Tight Tiny Teen Scouts 5
Turning the System On or Off
735 Reeds Avenue 737 & 739 Reeds Ave., Red Bluff, CA 96080 - MLS# 20240686 | CENTURY 21
Missing 2023 Showtimes Near Landmark Cinemas Peoria
NHS England » Winter and H2 priorities
Virginia New Year's Millionaire Raffle 2022
3476405416
north jersey garage & moving sales - craigslist
Dcf Training Number
Contracts for May 28, 2020
Providence Medical Group-West Hills Primary Care
yuba-sutter apartments / housing for rent - craigslist
At 25 Years, Understanding The Longevity Of Craigslist
Robotization Deviantart
Weather Underground Durham
Visit the UK as a Standard Visitor
Pioneer Library Overdrive
Vlacs Maestro Login
Motor Mounts
Bi State Schedule
Davita Salary
Shiftwizard Login Johnston
Beth Moore 2023
Craigslist List Albuquerque: Your Ultimate Guide to Buying, Selling, and Finding Everything - First Republic Craigslist
Craigslist Free Manhattan
Citibank Branch Locations In Orlando Florida
Windshield Repair & Auto Glass Replacement in Texas| Safelite
Yale College Confidential 2027
Caphras Calculator
26 Best & Fun Things to Do in Saginaw (MI)
Phone Store On 91St Brown Deer
The Pretty Kitty Tanglewood
Craigslist Sparta Nj
25100 N 104Th Way
Unpleasant Realities Nyt
Maurices Thanks Crossword Clue
The Goshen News Obituary
Lux Nails & Spa
Latest Posts
Article information

Author: Msgr. Benton Quitzon

Last Updated:

Views: 5973

Rating: 4.2 / 5 (43 voted)

Reviews: 82% of readers found this page helpful

Author information

Name: Msgr. Benton Quitzon

Birthday: 2001-08-13

Address: 96487 Kris Cliff, Teresiafurt, WI 95201

Phone: +9418513585781

Job: Senior Designer

Hobby: Calligraphy, Rowing, Vacation, Geocaching, Web surfing, Electronics, Electronics

Introduction: My name is Msgr. Benton Quitzon, I am a comfortable, charming, thankful, happy, adventurous, handsome, precious person who loves writing and wants to share my knowledge and understanding with you.